home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- LUCID DREAMING: AWAKE IN YOUR SLEEP?
-
- By Susan Blackmore
-
- From Skeptical Inquirer, Vol. 15 Summer 1991
- pages 362-370
-
- What could it mean to be conscious in your dreams? For most of us,
- dreaming is something quite separate from normal life. When we
- wake up from being chased by a ferocious tiger, or seduced by a
- devastatingly good-looking Nobel Prize winner we realize with
- relief or disappointment that "it was only a dream."
-
- Yet there are some dreams that are not like that. Lucid dreams are
- dreams in which you know at the time that you are dreaming. That
- they are different from ordinary dreams is obvious as soon as you
- have one. The experience is something like waking up in your
- dreams. It is as though you "come to" and find you are dreaming.
-
- Lucid dreams used to be a topic within psychical research and
- parapsychology. Perhaps their incomprehensibility made them good
- candidates for being thought paranormal. More recently, however,
- they have begun to appear in psychology journals and have dropped
- out of parapsychology - a good example of how the field of
- parapsychology shrinks when any of its subject matter is actually
- explained.
-
- Lucidity has also become something of a New Age fad. There are
- machines and gadgets you can buy and special clubs you can join to
- learn how to induce lucid dreams. But this commercialization
- should not let us lose sight of the very real fascination of lucid
- dreaming. It forces us to ask questions about the nature of
- consciousness, deliberate control over our actions, and the nature
- of imaginary worlds.
-
- A Real Dream or Not?
-
- The term lucid dreaming was coined by the Dutch psychiatrist
- Frederik van Eeden in 1913. It is something of a misnomer since it
- means something quite different from just clear or vivid dreaming.
- Nevertheless we are certainly stuck with it. Van Eeden explained
- that in this sort of dream "the re-integration of the psychic
- functions is so complete that the sleeper reaches a state of
- perfect awareness and is able to direct his attention, and to
- attempt different acts of free volition. Yet the sleep, as I am
- able confidently to state, is undisturbed, deep, and refreshing."
-
- This implied that there could be consciousness during sleep, a
- claim many psychologists denied for more than 50 years. Orthodox
- sleep researchers argued that lucid dreams could not possibly be
- real dreams. If the accounts were valid, then the experiences must
- have occurred during brief moments of wakefulness or in the
- transition between waking and sleeping, not in the kind of deep
- sleep in which rapid eye movements (REMs) and ordinary dreams
- usually occur. In other words, they could not really be dreams at
- all.
-
- This presented a challenge to lucid dreamers who wanted to
- convince people that they really were awake in their dreams. But
- of course when you are deep asleep and dreaming you cannot shout,
- "Hey! Listen to me. I'm dreaming right now." All the muscles of
- the body are paralyzed.
-
- It was Keith Hearne (1978), of the University of Hull, who first
- exploited the fact that not all the muscles are paralyzed. In REM
- sleep the eyes move. So perhaps a lucid dreamer could signal by
- moving the eyes in a predetermined pattern. Just over ten years
- ago, lucid dreamer Alan Worsley first managed this is in Hearne's
- laboratory. He decided to move his eyes left and right eight times
- in succession whenever he became lucid. Using a polygraph, Hearne
- could watch the eye movements for sign of the special signal. He
- found it in the midst of REM sleep. So lucid dreams are real
- dreams and do occur during REM sleep.
-
- Further research showed that Worsley's lucid dreams most often
- occurred in the early morning, around 6:30 A.M., nearly half an
- hour into a REM period and toward the end of a burst of rapid eye
- movements. They usually lasted for two to five minutes. Later
- research showed that they occur at times of particularly high
- arousal during REM sleep (Hearne 1978).
-
- It is sometimes said that discoveries in science happen when the
- time is right for them. It was one of those odd things that at
- just the same time, but unbeknown to Hearne, Stephen LaBerge, at
- Stanford University in California, was trying the same experiment.
- He too succeeded, but resistance to the idea was very strong. In
- 1980, both Science and Nature rejected his first paper on the
- discovery (LaBerge 1985). It was only later that it became clear
- what an important step this had been.
-
- An Identifiable State?
-
- It would be especially interesting if lucid dreams were associated
- with a unique physiological state. In fact this has not been
- found, although this is not very surprising since the same is true
- of other altered states, such as out-of-body experiences and
- trances of various kinds. However, lucid dreams do tend to occur
- in periods of higher cortical arousal. Perhaps a certain threshold
- of arousal has to be reached before awareness can be sustained.
-
- The beginning of lucidity (marked by eye signals, of course) is
- associated with pauses in breathing, brief changes in heart rate,
- and skin response changes, but there is no unique combination that
- allows the lucidity to be identified by an observer.
-
- In terms of the dream itself, there are several features that seem
- to provoke lucidity. Sometimes heightened anxiety or stress
- precedes it. More often there is a kind of intellectual
- recognition that something "dreamlike" or incongruous is going on
- (Fox 1962; Green 1968; LaBerge 1985).
-
- It is common to wake from an ordinary dream and wonder, "How on
- earth could I have been fooled into thinking that I was really
- doing push-ups on a blue beach?" A little more awareness is shown
- when we realize this in the dream. If you ask yourself, "Could
- this be a dream?" and answer "No" (or don't answer at all), this
- is called a pre-lucid dream. Finally, if you answer "Yes", it
- becomes a fully lucid dream.
-
- It could be that once there is sufficient cortical arousal it is
- possible to apply a bit of critical thought; to remember enough
- about how the world ought to be to recognize the dream world as
- ridiculous, or perhaps to remember enough about oneself to know
- that these events can't be continuous with normal waking life.
- However, tempting as it is to conclude that the critical insight
- produces the lucidity, we have only an apparent correlation and
- cannot deduce cause and effect from it.
-
- Becoming a Lucid Dreamer
-
- Surveys have show that about 50 percent of people (and in some
- cases more) have had at least one lucid dream in their lives.
- (see, for example, Blackmore 1982; Gackenbach and LaBerge 1988;
- Green 1968.) Of course surveys are unreliable in that many people
- may not understand the question. In particular, if you have never
- had a lucid dream, it is easy to misunderstand what is meant by
- the term. So overestimates might be expected. Beyond this, it
- does not seem that surveys can find out much. There are no very
- consistent differences between lucid dreamers and others in terms
- of age, sex, education, and so on (Green 1968; Gackenbach and
- LaBerge 1988).
-
- For many people, having lucid dream is fun, and they want to learn
- how to have more or to induce them at will. One finding from early
- experimental work was that high levels of physical (and emotional)
- activity during the day tend to precede lucidity at night. Waking
- during the night and carrying out some kind of activity before
- falling asleep again can also encourage a lucid dream during the
- next REM period and is the basis of some induction techniques.
-
- Many methods have been developed (Gackenbach and Bosveld 1989;
- Tart 1988; Price and Cohen 1988). They roughly fall into three
- categories.
-
- One of the best known is LaBerge's MILD (Mnemonic Induction of
- Lucid Dreaming). This is done on waking in the early morning from
- a dream. You should wake up fully, engage in some activity like
- reading or walking about, and then lie down to go to sleep again.
- Then you must imagine yourself asleep and dreaming, rehearse the
- dream from which you woke, and remind yourself, "Next time I dream
- this I want to remember I'm dreaming."
-
- A second approach involves constantly reminding yourself to become
- lucid throughout the day rather than the night. This is based on
- the idea that we spend most of our time in a kind of waking daze.
- If we could be more lucid in waking life, perhaps we could be more
- lucid while dreaming. German psychologist Paul Tholey suggests
- asking yourself many times every day, "Am I dreaming or not?" This
- sound easy but is not. It takes a lot of determination and
- persistence not to forget all about it. For those who do forget,
- French researcher Clerc suggests writing a large "C" on your hand
- (for "conscious") to remind you (Tholey 1983; Gackenbach and
- Bosveld 1989).
-
- This kind of method is similar to the age-old technique for
- increasing awareness by meditation and mindfulness. Advanced
- practitioners of meditation claim to maintain awareness through a
- large proportion of their sleep. TM is often claimed to lead to
- sleep awareness. So perhaps it is not surprising that some recent
- research finds association between meditation and increased
- lucidity (Gackenbach and Bosveld 1989).
-
- The third and final approach requires a variety of gadgets. The
- idea is to use some sort of external signal to remind people,
- while they are actually in REM sleep, that they are dreaming.
- Hearne first tried spraying water onto sleepers' faces or hands
- but found it too unreliable. This sometimes caused them to
- incorporate water imagery into their dreams, but they rarely
- became lucid. He eventually decided to use a mild electrical shock
- to the wrist. His "dream machine" detects changes in breathing
- rate (which accompany the onset of REM) and then automatically
- delivers a shock to the wrist (Hearne 1990).
-
- Meanwhile, in California, LaBerge was rejecting taped voices and
- vibrations and working instead with flashing lights. The original
- version was laboratory based and used a personal computer to
- detect the eye movements of REM sleep and to turn on flashing
- lights whenever the REMs reached a certain level. Eventually,
- however, all the circuitry was incorporated into a pair of
- goggles. The idea is to put the goggles on at night, and the
- lights will flash only when you are asleep and dreaming. The user
- can even control the level of eye movements at which the lights
- begin to flash.
-
- The newest version has a chip incorporated into the goggles. This
- will not only control the lights but will store data on
- eye-movement density during the night and when and for how long
- the lights were flashing, making fine tuning possible. At the
- moment, the first users have to join in workshops at LaBerge's
- Lucidity Institute and learn how to adjust the settings, but
- within a few months he hopes the whole process will be fully
- automated. (See LaBerge's magazine, DreamLight.)
-
- LaBerge tested the effectiveness of the Dream Light on 44 subjects
- who came into the laboratory, most for just one night. Fifty-five
- percent had at least one lucid dream this way. The results
- suggested that this method is about as succesful as MILD, but
- using the two together is the most effective (LaBerge 1985).
-
- Lucid Dreams as an Experimental Tool
-
- There are a few people who can have lucid dreams at will. And the
- increase in induction techniques has provided many more subjects
- who have them frequently. This has opened the way to using lucid
- dreams to answer some of the most interesting questions about
- sleep and dreaming.
-
- How long do dreams take? In the last century, Alfred Maury had a
- long and complicated dream that led to his being beheaded by a
- guillotine. He woke up terrified, and found that the headboard of
- his bed had fallen on his neck. From this, the story goes, he
- concluded that the whole dream had been created in the moment of
- awakening.
-
- This idea seems to have got into popular folklore but was very
- hard to test. Researchers woke dreamers at various stages of
- their REM period and found that those who had been longer in REM
- claimed longer dreams. However, accurate timing became possible
- only when lucid dreamers could send "markers" from the dream
- state.
-
- LaBerge asked his subjects to signal when they became lucid and
- then count a ten-second period and signal again. Their average
- interval was 13 seconds, the same as they gave when awake. Lucid
- dreamers, like Alan Worsley, have also been able to give accurate
- estimates of the length of whole dreams or dream segments
- (Schatzman, Worsley, and Fenwick 1988).
-
- Dream Actions
-
- As we watch sleeping animals it is often tempting to conclude that
- they are moving their eyes in response to watching a dream, or
- twitching their legs as they dream of chasing prey. But do
- physical movements actually relate to the dream events?
-
- Early sleep researchers occassionally reported examples like a
- long series of left-right eye movements when a dreamer had been
- dreaming of watching a ping-pong game, but they could do no more
- than wait until the right sort of dream came along.
-
- Lucid dreaming made proper experimentation possible, for the
- subjects could be asked to perform a whole range of tasks in their
- dreams. In one experiment with researchers Morton Schatzman and
- Peter Fenwick, in London, Worsley planned to draw large triangles
- and to signal with flicks of his eyes every time he did so. While
- he dreamed, the electromyogram, recording small muscle movements,
- showed not only the eye signals but spikes of electrical activity
- in the right forearm just afterward. This showed that the
- preplanned actions in the dream produced corresponding muscle
- movements (Schatzman, Worsley, and Fenwick 1988).
-
- Further experiments, with Worsley kicking dream objects, writing
- with umbrellas, and snapping his fingers, all confirmed that the
- muscles of the body show small movements corresponding to the
- body's actions in the dream. The question about eye movements was
- also answered. The eyes do track dream objects. Worsley could
- even produce slow scanning movements, which are very difficult to
- produce in the absence of a "real" stimulus (Schatzman, Worsley,
- and Fenwick 1988).
-
- LaBerge was especially interested in breathing during dreams.
- This stemmed from his experiences at age five when he had dreamed
- of being an undersea pirate who could stay under water for very
- long periods without drowning. Thirty years later he wanted to
- find out whether dreamers holding their breath in dreams do so
- physically as well. The answer was yes. He and other lucid
- dreamers were able to signal from the dream and then hold their
- breath. They could also breathe rapidly in their dreams, as
- revealed on the monitors. Studying breathing during dreamed
- speech, he found that the person begins to breathe out at the
- start of an utterance just as in real speech (LaBerge and Dement
- 1982a).
-
- Hemispheric Differences
-
- It is known that the left and right hemispheres are activated
- differently during different kinds of tasks. For example, singing
- uses the right hemisphere more, while counting and other, more
- analytical tasks use the left hemisphere more. By using lucid
- dreams, LaBerge was able to find out whether the same is true in
- dreaming.
-
- In one dream he found himself flying over a field. (Flying is
- commonly associated with lucid dreaming.) He signaled with his
- eyes and began to sing "Row, row, row your boat...." He then made
- another signal and counted slowly to ten before signaling again.
- The brainwave records showed just the same patterns of activation
- that you would expect if he had done these tasks while awake
- (LaBerge and Dement 1982b).
-
- Dream Sex
-
- Although it is not often asked experimentally, I am sure plenty of
- people have wondered what is happening in their bodies while they
- have their most erotic dreams.
-
- LaBerge tested a woman who could dream lucidly at will and could
- direct her dreams to create the sexual experiences she wanted.
- (What a skill!) Using appropriate physiological recording, he was
- able to show that her dream orgasms were matched by true orgasms
- (LaBerge, Greenleaf, and Kedzierski 1983).
-
- Experiments like these show that there is a close correspondence
- between actions of the dreamer and, if not real movements, at
- least eletrical responses. This puts lucid dreaming somewhere
- between real actions, in which muscles work to move the body, and
- waking imagery, in which they are rarely involved at all. So what
- exactly is the status of the dream world?
-
- The Nature of the Dream World
-
- It is tempting to think that the real world and the world of
- dreams are totally separate. Some of the experiments already
- mentioned show that there is no absolute dividing line. There are
- also plenty of stories that show the penetrability of the
- boundary.
-
- Alan Worsley describes one experiment in which his task was to
- give himself a prearranged number of small electric shocks by
- means of a machine measuring his eye movements. He went to sleep
- and began dreaming that it was raining and he was in a sleeping
- bag by a fence with gate in it. He began to wonder whether he was
- dreaming and thought it would be cheating to activate the shocks
- if he was awake. Then, while making the signals, he worried about
- the machine, for it was out there with him in the rain and might
- get wet (Schatzman, Worsley, and Fenwick 1988).
-
- This kind of interference is amusing, but there are dreams of
- confusion that are not. The most common and distinct are called
- false awakenings. You dream of waking up but in fact, of course,
- are still asleep. Van Eeden (1913) called these "wrong waking up"
- and described them as "demoniacal, uncanny, and very vivid and
- bright, with ... a strong diabolical light." The French zoologist
- Yves Delage, writing in 1919, described how he had heard a knock
- at his door and a friend calling for his help. He jumped out of
- bed, went to wash quickly with cold water, and when that woke him
- up he realized he had been dreaming. The sequence repeated four
- times before he finally actually woke up - still in bed.
-
- A student of mine described her infuriating recurrent dream of
- getting up, cleaning her teeth, getting dressed, and then cycling
- all the way to the medical school at the top of a long hill, where
- she finally would realize that she had dreamed it all, was late
- for lectures, and would have to do it all over again for real.
-
- The one positive benefit of false awakenings is that they can
- sometimes be used to induce out-of-body-experiences (OBEs).
- Indeed, Oliver Fox (1962) recommends this as a method for
- achieving the OBE. For many people OBEs and lucid dreams are
- practically indistinguishable. If you dream of leaving your body,
- the experience is much the same. Also recent research suggests
- that the same people tend to have both lucid dreams and OBEs
- (Blackmore 1988, Irwin 1988).
-
- All of these experiences have something in common. In all of them
- the "real" wolrd has been replaced by some kind of imaginary
- replica. Celia Green, of the Institute of Psychophysical Research
- at Oxford, refers to all such states as "metachoric experiences."
-
- Jayne Gackenbach, a psychologist from the University of Alberta,
- Canada, relates these experiences to UFO-abduction stories and
- near-death-experiences (NDEs). The UFO abductions are the most
- bizarre but are similar in that they too involve the replacement
- of the perceived world by a hallucinatory replica.
-
- There is an important difference between lucid dreams and these
- other states. In the lucid dream one has insight into the state
- (in fact that defines it). In false awakening, one does not (again
- by definition). In typical OBEs, people think they have really
- left their bodies. In UFO "abductions" they believe the little
- green men are "really there"; and in NDEs, they are convinced they
- are rushing down a real tunnel toward a real light and into the
- next world. It is only in the lucid dream that one realizes it is
- a dream.
-
- I have often wondered whether insight into these other experiences
- is possible and what the consequences might be. So far I don't
- have any answers.
-
- Waking Up
-
- The oddest thing about lucid dreams - and, to many people who have
- them, the most compelling - is how it feels when you wake up. Upon
- waking up from a normal dream, you usually think, "Oh, that was
- only a dream." Waking up from a lucid dream is more continuous. It
- feels more real, it feels as though you were conscious in the
- dream. Why is this? I think the reason can be found by looking at
- the mental models the brain constructs in waking, in ordinary
- dreaming, and in lucid dreams.
-
- I have previously argued that what seems real is the most stable
- mental model in the system at any time. In waking life, this is
- almost always the input-driven model, the one that is built up
- from the sensory input. It is firmly linked to the body image to
- make a stable model of "me, here, now." It is easy to decide that
- this represents "reality" while all the other models being used at
- the same time are "just imagination" (Blackmore 1988).
-
- Now consider an ordinary dream. In that case there are lots of
- models being built but no input-driven model. In addition there is
- no adequate self-model or body image. There is just not enough
- access to memory to construct it. This means, if my hypothesis is
- right, that whatever model is most stable at any time will seem
- real. But there is no recognizable self to whom it seems real.
- There will just be a series of competing models coming and going.
- Is this what dreaming feels like?
-
- Finally, we know from research that in the lucid dream there is
- higher arousal. Perhaps this is sufficient to construct a better
- model of self. It is one that includes such important facts as
- that you have gone to sleep, that you intended to signal with your
- eyes, and so on. It is also more similar to the normal waking self
- than those fleeting constructions of the ordinary dream. This, I
- suggest, is what makes the dream seem more real on waking up.
- Because the you who remembers the dream is more similar to the you
- in the dream. Indeed, because there was a better model of you, you
- were more conscious.
-
- If this is right, it means that lucid dreams are potentially even
- more interesting than we thought. As well as providing insight
- into the nature of sleep and dreams, they may give clues to the
- nature of consciousness itself.
-
- References
-
-
- Blackmore, S. J. 1982. Beyond the Body. London: Heinemann.
- --------- 1988. A Theory of lucid dreams and OBEs. In
- Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain, 373-387, ed.
- J. Gackenbach and S. LaBerge. New York: Plenum.
- Delage, Y. 1919. Le Reve. Paris: Les Presses Universitaires de
- France.
- Fox, O. 1962. Astral Projection. New York: University Books.
- Gackenbach, J., and J. Bosveld. 1989. Control Your Dreams.
- New York: Harper & Row.
- Gackenbach, J., and S. LaBerge, eds. 1988. Conscious Mind,
- Sleeping Brain. New York: Plenum.
- Green, C. E. 1968. Lucid Dreams. London: Hamish Hamilton.
- Hearne, K. 1978. Lucid Dreams: An Electrophysiological and
- Psychological Study. Unpublished Ph.D.
- thesis, University of Hull.
- --------- 1990. The Dream Machine. Northants: Aquarian.
- Irwin, H. J. 1988. Out-of-body experiences and dream lucidity:
- Empirical perspectives. In Conscious Mind,
- Sleeping Brain, 353-371, ed. J. Gackenbach
- and S. LaBerge. New York: Plenum.
- LaBerge, S. 1985. Lucid Dreaming. Los Angeles: Tarcher.
- LaBerge, S. and W. Dement. 1982a. Voluntary control of
- respiration during REM sleep. Sleep Research,
- 11:107.
- --------- 1982b. Lateralization of alpha activity for dreamed
- singing and counting during REM sleep.
- Psychophysiology, 19:331-332.
- LaBerge, S., W. Greenleaf, and B. Kerzierski. 1983.
- Physiological responses to dreamed sexual
- activity during lucid REM sleep.
- Psychophysiology, 20:454-455.
- Price, R. F., and D. B. Cohen. 1988. Lucid dream induction: An
- empirical evaluation. In Conscious Mind,
- Sleeping Brain, 105-134, ed. J. Gackenbach
- and S. LaBerge. New York: Plenum.
- Schatzman, M., A. Worsley, and P. Fenwick. 1988.
- Correspondence during lucid dreams between
- dreamed and actual events. In Conscious Mind,
- Sleeping Brain, 155-179, ed. J. Gackenbach
- and S. LaBerge. New York: Plenum.
- Tart, C. 1988. From spontaneous event to lucidity: A review of
- attempts to consciously control nocturnal
- dreaming. In Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain,
- 67-103, ed. J Gackenbach and S. LaBerge. New
- York: Plenum.
- Tholey, P. 1983. Techniques for controlling and manipulating
- lucid dreams. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
- 57:79-90.
- Van Eeden, F. 1913. A study of dreams. Proceedings of the
- Society for Psychical Research, 26:431-461.
-
- Susan J. Blackmore is with the Perceptual Systems Research
- Centre, Department of Psychology, University of Bristol, and
- the School of Social Sciences, University of Bath.